People tend to listen to big talkers, whether they are women or men. Still, more women prefer not to use assertive language, according to a new study by economics Professor Ketki Sheth.
The study published in Management Science was conducted with fellow economist Shanthi Manian at Washington State University. The findings show that participants in an experiment more often followed advice when the people giving the advice used assertive “cheap talk,” or statements that cannot be verified as true. Examples of such statements are often found in job seeking cover letters, such as “I have extremely strong problem-solving skills.”
The experiment participants followed the advice people gave at similar rates regardless of their gender — even though they thought other people would be less likely to follow the advice of the female leaders.
For the experiment, Sheth and Manian recruited about 1,000 people to play a difficult online game. Roughly half were UC Merced students and half were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk online crowdsourcing portal. The players were paired with either a male or female leader, who gave advice online on how to play the game to earn the biggest reward.
Everyone had the same interactions with their leaders, except for two features — the gender of the leader and the assertiveness of the leaders’ language. The same advice was couched in language that ranged from less assertive, using statements such as “You probably have better problem-solving skills than I do, but here is what I am thinking,” to more assertive, such as “If you listen to my advice, I can assure you that my skills and experiences will help you perform well in this game.”
While all the leaders gave the same good advice, the more assertive the leader’s statements were, the more people were likely to follow the advice.
“It was surprising. We didn’t see actual discrimination — the subjects themselves seemed to respond about the same to men and women,” Manian said. “Yet, after the experiment was over, and we asked the participants what they thought we’d find, many of them expected discrimination.”
The people paired with a female leader who used assertive cheap talk were just as likely to follow the advice as those paired with a male leader using the same language, even though most participants characterized such language as being more masculine.
After the game was played, the researchers asked the participants whether they thought the leaders’ gender would matter. People were much more likely to believe that others would follow the advice from a male leader than a female leader, even if the advice was identical.
They were also asked what type of language they would choose if they were leaders in the game. While most men and women avoided the least assertive language, men were 10 percentage points more likely to prefer the most assertive language option.
Sheth said the results raise questions about why women prefer less assertive language and what price they may pay for that aversion.
“The fact that the subjects expected discrimination suggests that it's hard for people to know when discrimination is going to happen,” said Sheth.